Linky and the Brain: April 30, 2013
/
We're doing some expanding here at the Neuroblog. In the next few months, readers will start to notice some new names at the top of posts. This expansion of our authorship is due to a newly formalized partnership with the Stanford-based science communication group NeuWrite West.
As part of this expansion, Nick Weiler and I will trade off authorship of a weekly feature, highlighting the science-related internet content that caught our eyes the previous week. I'd like to encourage folks out there to use the comments to jump in and share any items they enjoyed.
With this introduction, I'll debute entry #1 in our new link-sharing feature.
Absolutely incredible slow-motion video of barn owl hunting
This came to my attention both via the internets and several folks who shared it with me on Facebook. A stunning video of a barn owl using an auditory cue to strike at prey. The video starts out with a side perspective, but keep watching past the first example as the video switches to showing a bottom up angle that highlights the view of the descending owl from the perspective of the prey. Looking at the focused gaze of the swooping owl, with its outstretched talons drawing inexorably closer, I realize its probably a blessing that mice can't see so well.
http://laughingsquid.com/barn-owl-attacking-its-prey-in-slow-motion/
Interested in the neural mechanisms of how barn owls localize sound? How maps of visual and auditory space are aligned in the barn owl brain to provide a neural substrate for the terrible precision of the bird's ability to locate prey? I'll direct the curious to the work of Mark Konishi (Caltech), as well as Eric Knudsen (Stanford, my graduate advisor, former postdoc of Konishi).
The Evolution of the Country Mouse and the City Mouse
As usual, Carl Zimmer shows science writers how its done in his post highlighting research into "urban evolution". In particular the work of Jason Munshi-South (Baruch College), who studies evolutionary trajectories of white footed-mouse populations in, and around, NYC.
The art of the ambiguous conference poster abstract
In honor of the rapidly approaching SfN 2013 poster abstract deadline, Dr. Becca (@doc_becca) on writing an abstract in the absence of any data (original publication date, last year). A subject near and dear to my heart right now (damn you, preliminary data! I wish you were a fully-fleshed out scientific story already.)
http://scientopia.org/blogs/drbecca/2012/05/09/the-art-of-the-ambiguous-conference-poster-abstract/
U.S. Lawmaker Proposes New Criteria for Choosing NSF Grants
As a researcher personally funded by the NSF, this Science Insider news article gave me all sorts of feelings.
http://news.sciencemag.org/scienceinsider/2013/04/us-lawmaker-proposes-new-criteri-1.html?ref=hp
And some opposition to the proposed changes, from President Obama.
A Tale Of Mice And Medical Research, Wiped Out By A Superstorm
A more tragic topic - NPR covers the tragic losses suffered by Gord Fishell (and other researchers at NYU) when Superstorm Sandy caused flooding in an offsite animal facility.
Battlestar Pedagogica: Using Science Fiction to teach Science!
http://blogs.plos.org/scied/2013/04/29/battlestar-pedagogica-using-science-fiction-to-teach-science/
As an avid science fiction reader (and BSG viewer), I enjoyed reading this post on using science fiction to teach scientific concepts in the classroom. In my experiences chatting about neuroscience with non-neuroscientists (especially my computer science friends), I've found referencing sci-fi concepts to be a remarkably useful way to a) capture attention/interest and b) generate fascinating and complex discussions of current neuroscience.
Isaac Asimov Memorial Debate.
Neil deGrasse Tyson moderates the Isaac Asimov Memorial Debate. This years topic: Nothingness. The panel: Lawrence Krauss (theoretical physics, ASU), J. Richard Gott (astrophysics, Princeton), veteran science journalists Jim Holt (science journalist) and Charles Seife (science journalist), and Eve Silverstein (physics, Stanford). (via io9)
http://io9.com/must-watch-neil-degrasse-tyson-moderates-a-debate-on-n-481003403
Academic Fraud, a profile of social psychologist Diederik Stapel in the NYTimes Magazine.
A long read from the NYTimes Magazine, on Dutch social psychologist Diederik Stapel who fabricated results in at least 55 of his published articles. Written by Yudhijit Bhattacharjee. (via Cori Bargmann, @betenoire1)
Ending on a Happy Note: First Steps in Border Collie Sheepdog Training.
From the twitter account of @herdyshepherd1, training a border collie to herd sheep.
https://twitter.com/herdyshepherd1/status/328977686573756416
And that's all the links I've got for now folks. See you round the web. -Astra
So, it's mind control rodent army time at last, is it?
/When I first started graduate school, I told all my friends (most of whom were theater folk) that I was going off to become a neuroscientist so I could gather a great mind-control rodent army and thereby take over the world. Well, let's just say I had the normal graduate school experience and got distracted by the VERY early pilot testing phase of this project. (I study molecular differences between synapses.) But I am glad (and only a little jealous) to know that someone was getting busy while I was getting into microscopic esoterica.
Read MoreObama unveils BRAIN Initiative – to be helmed by Newsome and Bargmann
/This morning, President Obama announced his highly anticipated and much debated new initiative for Brain Research Through Advancing Innovative Neurotechnologies (or BRAIN) to invest in "transformative" new technology that may one day allow neuroscience researchers to map the vast, dynamic patterns of brain activity with unprecedented detail.
Read MoreDutch Physicist To Document Tenure Process, "From Idea to Science"
/Given the woeful state of obscurity in which most of us are working, I was excited this afternoon to read the first in a series of articles at Ars Technica by a Dutch physicist who plans to blog the entire process of building his research program as an assistant professor.
Read MorePh.D's in Press (January-February 2013)
/Part 4 in an occasional feature, highlighting recently published articles featuring an author (or authors) who is a current member of the Stanford Neuroscience Ph.D program. (Part 1, Part 2, Part 3)* Starting 2013 on a strong note, we have two papers with first authors from the Stanford Neuro PhD community. First, Logan Grosenick (Suppes and Deisseroth labs) presents new variants of the GraphNet fMRI analysis method. Second, Jordan Nechvatal (Lyons lab) reviews brain imaging studies on the effect of stress exposure therapy for anxiety disorders (including phobias and PTSD).
Logan Grosenick
Grosenick, Klingenberg, Katovich, Knutson and Taylor. Interpretable whole-brain prediction analysis with GraphNet. Neuroimage, 2013 Jan 5. pii: S1053-8119(12)01248-7. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.12.062. [Epub ahead of print]. (Link)
Abstract: Multivariate machine learning methods are increasingly used to analyze neuroimaging data, often replacing more traditional "mass univariate" techniques that fit data one voxel at a time. In the functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) literature, this has led to broad application of "off-the-shelf" classification and regression methods. These generic approaches allow investigators to use ready-made algorithms to accurately decode perceptual, cognitive, or behavioral states from distributed patterns of neural activity. However, when applied to correlated whole-brain fMRI data these methods suffer from coefficient instability, are sensitive to outliers, and yield dense solutions that are hard to interpret without arbitrary thresholding. Here, we develop variants of the Graph-constrained Elastic Net (GraphNet), a fast, whole-brain regression and classification method developed for spatially and temporally correlated data that automatically yields interpretable coefficient maps (Grosenick et al., 2009b). GraphNet methods yield sparse but structured solutions by combining structured graph constraints (based on knowledge about coefficient smoothness or connectivity) with a global sparsity-inducing prior that automatically selects important variables. Because GraphNet methods can efficiently fit regression or classification models to whole-brain, multiple time-point data sets and enhance classification accuracy relative to volume-of-interest (VOI) approaches, they eliminate the need for inherently biased VOI analyses and allow whole-brain fitting without the multiple comparison problems that plague mass univariate and roaming VOI ("searchlight") methods. As fMRI data are unlikely to be normally distributed, we (1) extend GraphNet to include robust loss functions that confer insensitivity to outliers, (2) equip them with "adaptive" penalties that asymptotically guarantee correct variable selection, and (3) develop a novel sparse structured Support Vector GraphNet classifier (SVGN). When applied to previously published data (Knutson et al., 2007), these efficient whole-brain methods significantly improved classification accuracy over previously reported VOI-based analyses on the same data (Grosenick et al., 2008; Knutson et al., 2007) while discovering task-related regions not documented in the original VOI approach. Critically, GraphNet estimates fit to the Knutson et al. (2007) data generalize well to out-of-sample data collected more than three years later on the same task but with different subjects and stimuli (Karmarkar et al., submitted for publication). By enabling robust and efficient selection of important voxels from whole-brain data taken over multiple time points (>100,000 "features"), these methods enable data-driven selection of brain areas that accurately predict single-trial behavior within and across individuals.
Jordan Nechvatal
Nechvatal and Lyons. Coping changes the brain. Front. Behav. Neurosci., 22 February 2013 | doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2013.00013. (Link)
Abstract: One of the earliest and most consistent findings in behavioral neuroscience research is that learning changes the brain. Here we consider how learning as an aspect of coping in the context of stress exposure induces neuroadaptations that enhance emotion regulation and resilience. A systematic review of the literature identified 15 brain imaging studies in which humans with specific phobias or post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) were randomized to stress exposure therapies that diminished subsequent indications of anxiety. Most of these studies focused on functional changes in the amygdala and anterior corticolimbic brain circuits that control cognitive, motivational, and emotional aspects of physiology and behavior. Corresponding structural brain changes and the timing, frequency, and duration of stress exposure required to modify brain functions remain to be elucidated in future research. These studies will advance our understanding of coping as a learning process and provide mechanistic insights for the development of new interventions that promote stress coping skills.
*Regarding the mechanics of this feature: This is purely through the magic of an ongoing My NCBI search for the names of Neuro PhD students. I wouldn't be surprised if there were some false negatives in the data set. Neuro students - let me know if I've missed your paper, and I'll gladly add it.
Airplane Brain Quiz!
/Here's a tidbit I feel like sharing: Yesterday, I was flying from LA to SF - the last leg of a excruciating 22-hour journey back home from Israel. During that 15 minute window before landing, when my kindle had to be turned off just in case its electric presence flummoxed my Southwest airplane, I flipped through the inflight magazine.
And found this gem of sort-of neuroscience: a Brain Quiz (aka an advert for something in a pill bottle called "AlphaBrain". The website for AlphaBrain is so full of dubious neuro-technobabble that I'm categorically refusing to provide a link.)
With a befuddlement mostly provided by substantial amounts of jetlag (still feeling it. woohoo), I stared longest at question 3:
Ready for the answers? Curious which one of the rightmost boxes could possibly be the "most accurate association" with GABA (the major inhibitory neurotransmitter that is involved in just about everything)?
Here it goes.
Acetylcholine:
Mental speed, focus, memory. Commentary: Uh, I guess so. But maybe also muscle movements, seeing as how acetylcholine is THE transmitter at the neuromuscular junction. And I'm not too sure how what "mental speed" means, but acetylcholine is involved in attention, which I guess could work with the focus thing. And screwing with acetylcholine does affect learning/memory/plasticity, so I guess that's fine. Whatever.
Serotonin:
Positive mood. Commentary: Did you know that the vast majority of serotonin release is in the gastrointestinal tract, where it regulates intestinal movements? Mis-regulated intestinal movements sure leaves me in a bad mood. But sure, in the brain, release of serotonin does regulate mood. Drugs that increase serotonin levels in the brain are prescribed as antidepressants (e.g. selective serotonin re-uptake inhibitors, SSRI's), or used (and abused) as psychedelics (e.g. LSD, mescaline, MDMA).
Dopamine:
Coordination, pleasure, mental drive. Commentary: Pleasure? Ugh. Try "reward-driven learning". Does the coordination come from the loss of movement accompanying the death of dopaminergic neurons in Parkinson's disease? Not really a loss of coordination, so much as a categorical degeneration of motor control. "Mental drive" likely refers to the deficits in mental acuity, attention, and memory that accompany dopaminergic cell loss in Parkinson's. Also, reduced dopamine concentrations have been associated with ADHD, which could be characterized by less "mental drive". I guess. Maybe.
GABA:
Relaxation, sense of calm. Commentary: GABA, aka gamma-aminobutyric acid, aka the main inhibitory neurotransmitter in the CNS. It's diverse roles, reduced to the fact that many potent anesthetics are either GABA receptor agonists or positive modulators (e.g. alcohol, valium). Oh well. Note: for those interested in the differences in GABAergic inhibition between awake and anesthetized states, I direct you to a great recent publication by Michael Hausser and Matteo Carandini. First author Bilal Halder shows that in the mouse visual system, synaptic inhibition was substantially stronger in awake animals, when compared with anesthetized animals. A fun finding, given the (radically oversimplified) hypothesis that anesthetics work by increasing inhibition within the CNS. Insert spirited discussion about the difference between general changes in GABAergic tone (produced by anesthetics) and temporally/spatially/neuron specific synaptic inhibition (observed in awake conditions, likely disrupted by anesthetics).
Citation: Halder, Hausser and Carandini (2013). "Inhibition dominates sensory responses in the awake cortex." Nature 492, 97-100. Link.
BAM! Obama plans billions of dollars in funding for NIH Brain Activity Map initiative
/BAM! Obama plans billions of dollars in funding for NIH Brain Activity Map initiative
Read MoreTrue Facts about the Elephant Seal
/Recently, my lab decided to ditch the whole “doing science on a Friday” thing, and instead, go on a field trip. Roughly 1.5 hours from Stanford lies Ano Nuevo, a California State Park, and home of the largest mainland breeding colony of northern elephant seals in the entire world.(1)
In celebration of a fantastic afternoon filled with elephant seal babies, battles, and breeding, some photos of the Ano Nuevo colony, accompanied by some facts about the elephant seal.
[gallery columns="1" type="slideshow" ids="2525,2535,2537,2539,2549,2551,2547,2559,2545,2543,2553,2555,2561,2557,2563"]
Some Facts(2), Including Facts from Peer-Reviewed Journal Articles(3)
There are two species of elephant seals, northern and southern. Northern elephant seals hang out in the North Pacific, ranging from Baja California to Alaska. Southern elephant seals, being aptly named, inhabit the sub-Antarctic and Artic waters.
Northern elephant seals are very large. The only seal bigger than a northern elephant seal is a southern elephant seal. They can be mistaken for very large logs (this, I have done). Adult males can grow to over 13 feet, 4,500 pounds; females generally weigh in at 10 feet, 1,500 pounds.
A “seal” can belong to one of three families of fin-footed mammals: Odobenidae (walruses), Otariidae (eared seals, e.g. sea lions), or Phocidae (earless, or true seals). Elephant seals are true seals - they don’t have external ear flaps, and they get around while on land by throwing themselves along the ground. It’s hysterical to watch, until you realize that 4,500 pounds of seal is throwing itself at you, at a rate of up 8 miles an hour.
For most of the year, elephant seals are solitary animals, spending most of their time migrating. Ano Nuevo female elephant seals, fitted with satellite tracking equipment, have ventured as far north as Alaska, and as far west as the International Date Line.(4)
The only natural predators of the elephant seals are great white sharks and orcas. Downside: you’re someone’s idea of a tasty snack. Upside: at least it’s an apex predator.
During the 19th century, humans hunted the elephant seal nearly to extinction, for their blubber (used for lamp oil, similarly to whale blubber). Massive conservation efforts, and the invention of electricity, have restored population numbers from less than 100 seals in 1910, to approximately 150,000 today.(5)
During the breeding season, elephant seals throw a beach party during which time males fight to establish dominance, females give birth and then mate with the dominant males. During this time, the elephant seals will abstain from both food and water.
The “elephant” part of the elephant seal’s name is not a comment about its size. Instead, it refers to the adult male elephant seals nose (or proboscis), which, if you want to be excessively polite about it, looks like an elephants trunk. Dominant males inflate their noses to produce a noise that sounds like a cross between a stalling chain saw and an elephant with irritable bowel syndrome.(6)
[quicktime]http://www.stanford.edu/group/neurostudents/cgi-bin/wordpress/wp-content/uploads/2013/02/MVI_4945.mov[/quicktime]
The proboscis isn’t merely the elephant seal’s attempt to win the animal kingdoms Ugliest Mammal Award, the enlarged nose contains highly convoluted nasal cavities (measuring up to 3140 cm2 in an adult male); this enlarged surface area allows elephant seals to reabsorb enough moisture from their exhalations to maintain water balance during the extended fast of the breeding season.(7)
Despite being mammals, and thus needing air to breath, elephant seals spend most of their time deep underwater – 91% of their time at sea is spent diving Our Ano Nuevo docent stated that the movement of an elephant seal descending underwater is be best described as “the same motion as a leaf on the wind”. A 4,500 pound leaf.(8) An “integrative hierarchical Bayesian state-space” model of Southern elephant seal movements, used to quantify how environmental factors influence an individual seal’s movement, is a thing that exists.(9)
Elephant seals hunt deep underwater, where light is scarce. Elephant seals are not equipped with echolocation, one very useful way to find stuff to eat when hunting in very dark water (see: whales). Instead, elephant seals have adapted their vision to be highly sensitive to low intensity light, with peak sensitivity at 485 nm. Coincidentally, 485 nm is the wavelength of bioluminescence produced by the southern elephant seal’s main prey: myctophid fish.(10)
Lastly, antibodies against the parasite Toxoplasma gondii(11) have been detected in Southern elephant seals.(12) Make of that what you will.
Footnotes:
1. For more on Ano Nuevo, including park and colony history and visitor information, go their excellent website. Back to text
2. Source: Marine Mammal Center; National Geographic. Back to text
3. Methods: A Pubmed search for Mirounga generated an extensive list of journal articles relating to elephant seals. Journal articles were selected from said list on the basis the level of awesomeness evident in the abstract. Back to text
4. Source: Robinson et al (2012). Foraging behavior and success of a mesopelagic predator in the northeast Pacific Ocean: insights from a data-rich species, the norther elephant seal. PLoS One. 7(5):e36728. Back to text
5 Source: Marine Mammal Center. Back to text
6. Go home evolution, you are drunk. Back to text
7. Source: Huntley et al (1984). The contribution of nasal countercurrent heat exchange to water balance in the northern elephant seal, Mirounga angustirostris. J Exp Biol 113:447-54. Back to text
8. Which is less like a leaf on the wind, an elephant seal, or Walsh, piloting Serenity? Thinking about which option just made you sadder? (This joke is dedicated to K.Bryant) Back to text
9. Source: Bestley et al (2013). Integrative modeling of animal movement: incorporating in situ habitat and behavioural information for a migratory marine predator. Proc Biol Sci. 280(1750):20122262. Back to text
10. Nicely done, evolution. Source: Vacquie-Garcia et al (2012). Foraging in the darkness of the Southern Ocean: Influence of bioluminescence of a deep diving predator. PLoS One: 7(8):e43565. Back to text
11. Let Neuro Ph.D candidate Patrick House remind you all about Toxoplasma gondii. Back to text
12. Source: Rengifo-Herrera et al (2012). Detection of Toxoplasma gondii antibodies in Antarctic pinnipeds. Vet Parasitol: 190(1-2):259-62. Back to text
Day in the (Grad School) Life: White eggs, brown eggs [updated with results]
/[Updated 2/7/13. Click here to skip to results of the hatch] So, I work in a chicken lab.*
What this involves:
- a weekly delivery of fertilized eggs from a farm located in California's Central Valley;
- storing the egg delivery in a wine fridge set to 55 degrees celsius
- two times a week, placing a set of fertilized eggs in an industrial incubator for 3 weeks
- waiting for a cheeping flock to chicks to hatch
When I first joined my lab, we exclusively used white eggs - ones hatched by white leghorns.
In recent months however, the farm has been sending us eggs that look like this:
Clearly, these are not eggs hatched by a white leghorn (leghorn eggs are described as "pearl white" Source: Henderson's Chicken Breed Chart).
Now, I guess I could go to the internet, and carefully research the egg coloration/patterns of common chicken breeds. If I did that, I would probably be able to narrow down the potential breed of chicken currently growing inside the eggs pictured above. But that would be boring.**
Instead, I'm waiting until Wednesday, when the first batch of mystery brown eggs is scheduled to hatch. I'm betting the chicks in the uniformly brown eggs are either Rhode Islands, the most common layer of brown eggs. As for the speckled eggs, they may be eggs laid by the same breed as the uniformly brown eggs, or they could represent an additional breed.
Will all the chicks be the same breed, despite the range in egg coloration? What will that breed be? I'll be finding out (hopefully) on Wednesday.
In conclusion, to quote a post-doc with whom I've been discussing our inability to acquire white eggs: "Aw yeah science!"
------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------
*Technically, I'm a senior graduate student in the lab of Dr. Eric Knudsen, studying neural mechanisms underlying visual attention in the avian optic tectum. For the historical minded: Eric's lab has a long history of working with another avian model, the barn owl. In recent years, the focus in his lab has begun to shift to work in chickens. Yes, the barn owl is a much more majestic bird than the chicken. **For the scientific aspects of my research, it doesn't really matter what breed of chicken I'm using. Yes, it would be more elegant to use only one breed of bird, but doing experiments with brown eggs is better than not doing experiments with non-existant white eggs.
[Update 2/7/13 - the results of the Brown Egg Hatch]
[gallery type="slideshow" ids="2709,2711,2713,2715,2717,2719,2721,2723,2727"]